Understanding Memory: How Reliable Are Our Recollections?
Written on
Chapter 1: The Nature of Memory
Recent studies in memory have indicated that our recollections may often be less dependable than we believe. In a previous article titled "Mental Events Are More Real Than Reality Itself," I delved deeply into the mechanics of memory formation. The title reflects profound implications regarding how the reactivation of memory—and visualization—can relate to the Law of Attraction, offering ways to heal from trauma and create a more positive existence. Here, I aim to expand on these themes and introduce the concept of flashbulb memories. Although I won't extensively discuss the Law of Attraction in this piece, understanding memory will enhance your appreciation of its complexities and might even instill a sense of humility regarding the trust we place in our memories.
This realization also presents positive aspects worth considering. My recent explorations into behavioral neuroscience are inspired by an intriguing book I was invited to review: "Overloaded: How Every Aspect of Your Life is Influenced by Your Brain Chemicals" by Ginny Smith.
What Are Flashbulb Memories?
Flashbulb memories are a unique type of emotional memory created during highly charged events, often traumatic in nature. Ginny Smith provides notable examples, such as the death of Princess Diana and the September 11 terrorist attacks. Many of us can vividly recall our actions, locations, and even what we were wearing during such events. We remember peculiar details: the scents in the air, the subtle expressions of those around us, and even the specific shades of minor objects in a room.
For instance, I vividly recall my fifth birthday, even though my family did not celebrate such occasions as Jehovah's Witnesses. I remember racing up the stairs from our basement in an ecstatic rush, eager to announce my age to the world. At the time, I believed these details were infallible, yet recent research reveals that flashbulb memories may not be as reliable as we think.
The Process of Memory Formation
In my last article, I explained how memories are initially formed in both the hippocampus and the neocortex. Initially, these cortical traces are fixed; however, the hippocampus has limited capacity due to the fewer neurons located deep within the brain. Over time, as we encounter new information, it reactivates these cortical neurons, allowing them to adapt and change, creating a flexible memory state.
This adaptability raises concerns about the reliability of eyewitness testimony in legal contexts. Elizabeth Loftus's research has shown how leading questions can significantly alter a person's memory during cross-examination, demonstrating the fragility of our recollections.
Misremembering Significant Events
For a clearer example of how this phenomenon works—even with what we might consider a dependable memory—Ginny Smith references the events of 9/11. Many individuals, including former President George W. Bush, altered their recollections over time. For instance, numerous people assert they first learned of the attacks via television, despite the absence of footage of the initial crash at the time. Instead of remembering "like it was yesterday," one might say they recall it as "remembering like it was tomorrow," highlighting how our memories can be reshaped by subsequent information.
I, too, believed I learned about the attacks during French class, but my brother recalls being woken up by our father that morning, contradicting my memory. This discrepancy raises questions about the accuracy of our recollections and the possible influences at play.
Explaining the Mandela Effect
This discussion of flashbulb memories and confabulation sheds light on what is commonly referred to as the Mandela Effect. While I don’t dismiss the peculiar experiences people report, I believe understanding memory processes demystifies many claims. It illustrates how individuals can genuinely hold opposing views of historical events, believing they are providing accurate accounts.
As Ginny Smith notes, "With our current understanding of memory formation in the brain, it is easy to see how this happens." A few days post-event, exposure to media could cause one’s neurons to enter a flexible state, allowing the new information to alter the original memory. Conversations and other inputs can similarly reshape our recollections, leading to an unreliable narrative of past events.
Conclusion: Harnessing Memory’s Complexity
While the implications of these findings can be unsettling, they also offer pathways to harness memory's complexities for positive outcomes. In upcoming discussions, I will explore how we can leverage this neurological process for beneficial purposes. I invite you to share your thoughts below: Do you have any flashbulb memories? After reading this article, do you feel less certain about particular recollections? To what extent should we trust our memories, and how much skepticism is reasonable? I look forward to hearing your insights.